Are fakers outliers or bellwethers?

Well, the new digs here at ScienceBlogs have thin walls (GrrlScientist, will you please turn down that stereo!), which means that sometimes we get sucked into the conversations our neighbors are having. And, almost as if this were the complex at Melrose Place (shut up!), a lot of us have been chattering about the same people, notably Hwang Woo Suk.

So, for example, I've been hearing Chris Mooney telling his guests that, peer review or no peer review, the community of scientists will always include some fakers. Through the air-vent, I've got PZ Myers musing on how detection (or not) of the fakers could be connected to how well-established or cutting-edge the faked research seems to be.

As it happens, I've always taken thin walls as an excuse to poke my head into a conversation, so here's my take on fakers and the mechanisms within the tribe of science for dealing with them.

The title of Chris's post is There Will Always Be Fakers, which is a claim I'm hearing more and more lately. Indeed, here's what Derek Lowe says about fakery in science:

[S]cientists do indeed realize that fraud happens, because every working scientist has seen it. For starters, most large academic departments have tales of grad students or post-docs whose work could never be trusted. And all of us in research have run into papers in the literature whose techniques won't reproduce, no matter what, and the suspicions naturally grow that they were the product of exaggeration or wishful thinking. The number of possible publications sins alone is huge: yields of chemical reactions get padded, claims of novelty and generality get inflated, invalidating research from other labs doesn't get cited.

It's painful for me to admit it, but this kind of thing goes on all the time. And as long as the labs are staffed with humans, we're not going to be able to get rid of it. The best we can do is discourage it and correct it when we can.

The main issue that gets disputed when talking about fakers in lab coats is just how common they are. Are they a tiny proportion of the tribe of science, or are they as common as (say) cheaters in a business ethics class? We don't know. To the best of my knowledge, there haven't been any studies that have turned up reliable data on the incidence of fakery, the proportion of the scientific literature that is rendered unreliable by such fakery, etc. Which means a scientist who says that only a tiny number of fraudulent papers make it into the scientific literature is, essentially, making a claim with no data to support it.

Even if human nature makes it inevitable that some scientists are drawn to dishonesty, remember that science strives to be a fact-based community, making fakery much more of a problem than poor table manners or B.O. Given the particular epistemic task to which scientist set themselves (building a good picture of how the various bits of the universe work), there's no level of fakery (except "none") that you can really label "acceptable" -- at least, it's not acceptable to let it continue and infiltrate the literature, grant applications, and the like. Scientists want the facts. Those who try to pass off made-up stuff as facts are undermining the enterprise and Must Be Stopped. Take away their licenses to practice science. Zap their funding. Shun them. Assign them a minder. But whatever the means of stopping them, once fakers have been detected, they cannot be allowed to continue their wrong doing, or else they'll wreck it (the body of reliable scientific knowledge) for everyone else.

OK, so how do you detect the cheaters? Peer review is supposed to be one mechanism: your manuscript doesn't get published until it survives the scrutiny of a subset of your scientific peers. These peers are supposed to figure out whether the experiments you describe are good ones, whether your analysis of your data is appropriate, whether your interpretation of the results is resonable. Presumably, if they sniff out results that are too good to be true (including figures or graphs that look doctored), they're supposed to ask some hard questions about them. The fact that people who know a lot about systems like the one you work with will be subjecting your manuscript to this kind of scrutiny is supposed to deter people from cheating.

But there are a couple factors that can make this more complicated than you might expect. First off, the more cutting-edge your research is, the less likely that there is a large number of other scientists in your field who have had success with the kinds of systems you study or the kinds of things you're doing with them -- which means, there will be fewer scientist who could give the same kind of well-grounded peer review of your manuscript. Indeed, the reviewers may be in a tricky spot: they could be credulous ("I've never seen anything like this before, but it's all so new that maybe they really did see it.") or curmudgeonly ("It doesn't fit with what I know, so I don't like it, not one bit!"). The latter mode may keep new findings that really are valid (and useful, and important) out of the literature, while the former may let crap into the literature. Simply because we have less experience with the newer lines of research, these may be more likely, when they hit the journals, to be riddled with errors. Some of these errors are inevitable when scientists are working in new areas; less experience means it's harder even for the researchers to distinguish the good results from the errors. But some of the problems could be the result of fraud.

In areas of research that are not at the very frontiers of science, peer reviewers have more collective experience to draw on in order to evaluate manuscripts. They have more of a gut feeling for results that seem reasonable and those that seem fishy. So, you might think it would be harder to fake results the closer the area of research gets to "textbook" knowledge. But, as PZ writes:

Hwang Woo Suk's results were not at all unexpected, did not contradict any accepted scientific concepts, and were dramatic because they represented a methodological breakthrough. In a way, it was almost a "safe" category in which to cheat: lots of people are trying to transform adult cell nuclei into totipotent stem cells, it looks like a problem that's just going to require a lot of trial-and-error hammering to resolve, and what Dr Hwang did was steal priority on a result he could anticipate would be "replicated" (or more accurately, done for the first time) in short order. This is one of the hardest categories of science to police, I would think. It's frontier science all right, but it's only one step beyond the textbook.

In other words, working in a realm of science where most of the scientists agree about how things go (or how things will go once technique is perfected) may make it easier to cheat. Just as peer reviewers know what a reasonable result should look like, so do researchers ... and they might be tempted to take the short-cut of just making up reasonable looking results rather than trying to wring them out of the experimental system.

What a lot of non-scientists don't realize about peer reviewing of scientific manuscripts is that it does not generally involve the reviewers attempting to replicate the experiments reported in the manuscripts. Peer reviewers who happen to be working on the same experimental systems may try to get the same results, but seeing a papar in the journal is no guarantee that this has happened.

But, it's only a matter of time before someone does try to replicate the results reported in the journals, right? If someone has managed to get faked results past peer review, they are bound to be exposed eventually, right?

Depends how many other scientists see the experiment and/or results reported in the paper as a useful starting point for further work. It also depends on how finicky an experimental system it is. Sometimes reproducing an experimental result is hard because you don't have the same skill in the lab as the team who reported it. Or, you can afford the new, reliable equipment (or high-quality reagents) that team used. Or there might be subtle but important experimental factors not reported in the paper (because they weren't recognized as important) that need to be just so for the experiment to go. (For the chemical reactions I studied, reactor geometry and stirring speed were the two variables that were underreported in the literature, and this made months of my life suck.)

Also, scientists might abandon an experimental system if they don't have the patience required to obtain special reagents from very busy scientists in competing research labs.

The bottom line is that there are plenty of published results that don't undergo anything like a straight replication by other research groups. Given that there seems to be very little professional reward for replicating other groups' published results, this probably won't change any time soon. And this means that the literature may have lots of problems -- whether the result of fraud or honest mistakes -- mixed right in with all that science-y goodness.

It seems, therefore, that rooting out problems (fakery and honest mistakes) might be better accomplished before the manuscripts even go out to the journal editors. Chris suggests that ratting out your collaborator/colleague/underling/boss might be the best way to make happen. But, there are power dynamics to navigate. How much standing does a student have when it's the boss who's faking data? Does the department chair believe some newbie (still learning how to use all the equipment, for goodness sake) over an esteemed and highly productive member of the department? Should scientific collaborators subject each other to surprise inspections? Sure, science is an enterprise in which the community's fortunes rise or fall based on the honesty of its members, and to a certain extent that makes each scientist his or her scientific sibling's keeper. But is a presumption of guilt really going to be the thing to help the scientific community get fakery under control?

A response I like better would be taking steps to strengthen scientists' awareness of their connectedness to each other. Rather than a consciousness-raising that makes each scientist aware that every other scientist is potentially a lying rat-bastard, there needs to be a consciousness-raising about what the whole point of scientific activity was supposed to be in the first place: not tenure, awards, big buckets of grant money, or dates, but a better and better understanding of how the stuff in the universe works. You may be able to get the tenure, the fame, the bucks, and the booty with lies, but you can't get the better understanding with fakery. And maybe that means the scientific establishment needs to pull back from its current structure of staging gladiatorial battles for limited resources (tenure/fame/bucks/booty).

You might wonder if the number of scientific fakers detected in recent months is a testament not to the vigilance of the scientific community in keeping its knowledge base sound, but to an increase in the number of "scientists" opting to cut to the chase by seeking the rewards of science (tenure/fame/bucks/booty) without getting mired in the slow, difficult processes involved in actually doing science. Maybe bigger rewards for scientific achievement make the temptation to fake such achievements harder to resist. (I often buy a lottery ticket when the jackpot tops $80 million.)

The scientific community needs to get itself to the point where it is absolutely unthinkable for any of its members to sacrifice the overarching goal (getting good knowledge) for an external reward. It needs to create and maintain an environment where fakery does nothing good (even in the short term) for anyone who might perpetrate it.

The trick, of course, is figuring out how to get there from here.

More like this

Predatory open access journals seem to be a hot topic these days. In fact, there seems to be kind of a moral panic surrounding them. I would like to counter the admittedly shocking and scary stories around that moral panic by pointing out that perhaps we shouldn't be worrying so much about a fairly…
tags: book review, Plastic Fantastic, How the Biggest Fraud in Physics Shook the Scientific World, physics, ethics, fraud, Bell Labs, Lucent Technologies, Jan Hendrik Schön, Eugenie Samuel Reich Physicist Jan Hendrik Schön was too good to be true. After graduating from the University of Konstanz…
The Hwang Woo Suk stem cell research scandal has triggered quite a bit of concerned introspection in the scientific community. Orac has some useful comments on a good article in the NY Times that makes the distinction between "frontier science" and "textbook science", where much of the current stem…
Rethinking Peer Review: In reality, peer review is a fairly recent innovation, not widespread until the middle of the twentieth century. In the nineteenth century, many science journals were commandingly led by what Ohio State University science historian John C. Burnham dubbed "crusading and…

I discussed this, to no great resolution here. I think the trick is to make the payoff of fraud so low and so hard to attain that it is cheaper just to do good science.

Each paper is also supposed to make a conceptual change, that it is A and not B (while until now we thought B was more likely, or had no way to distinguish between A and B, or never knew that A was a possibility, etc.).

No need to replicate the experiment or even use the same system. If you trust a paper that says it is A and build your project on the assumption that A is correct, then your research is going to reveal if the original paper was correct or not.

I imagine (like you I'm operating without data) that the most common form of scientific fraud is not a person who fakes a major breakthrough for the sake of fame, money and groupies, but a person who is basically trying to be deadwood. They want to pad their tenure file with nondescript results in marginal journals, hoping that the tenure committee will not know enough about the field to recognize fluff. Really, they want to go from doing very little work to doing absolutely no work.

The "striving to be deadwood" crowd are also the kind of people who put no effort into teaching, but avoid criticism from students by giving them all As. I imagine a big tactic they use is to try to make their results so boring that no one will try to replicate them, look at their lab books, or send them email while they are on vacation.

Changing the payoff for major fraud or emphasizing the real goal of science probably won't effect this kind of fraud, but I imagine the collective drain of all of this deadwood could be quite high. Theory articulation is largely a matter of filling in boxes in a table that has already been established. If most of those boxes are filled in with numbers that seem reasonable, but have no backing, what good is the theory?

Another important thing is to start earlier. I went to a fairly small school (1,200 students), and I know at least one chem major who cheated his way thru ochem. He bribed a TA to find out his unknown for the last lab then matched up NMR pieces until he got an NMR that matched the known spectrum in the big book (I forget the name -- haven't done chem for several years now). Who knows what else he cheated at. I once tried to tell a prof, and she kind of laughed at me. I certainly wasn't taken seriously. As long as these situations are allowed to permeat through all levels, we will see PhD students who have been bluffing it all the way along or who think it's okay to cheat on the dissertation.

"The bottom line is that there are plenty of published results that don't undergo anything like a straight replication by other research groups."

I totally agree with this, and it does worry me. One thing that might help is to encourage the publication of these sorts of studies. Many of them will be "negative results", in the sense that they just confirm previous results without adding much new. There are a few journals providing an outlet. If you'll excusae the blatant plug:
Journal of Negative Results � Ecology and Evolutionary Biology
Journal of Negative Results in Biomedicine
Journal of Negative Observations in Genetic Oncology
Journal of Negative Results Speech and Audio Sciences

The problem is getting people to appreciate the value of this sort of work enough to put in the effort to write the paper up: the long-term benefits for science should be apparent, but it's less clear how to improve the short-term benefit for a scientist.

Bob

No need to replicate the experiment or even use the same system. If you trust a paper that says it is A and build your project on the assumption that A is correct, then your research is going to reveal if the original paper was correct or not.

A while ago, an online acquaintance of mine suggested suggested this exercise:

Go ten years back and pick an issue of Nature. Use the papers that appear in that issue as a small sample. Now see:
1) How many times those papers are subsequently cited; and,
2) How many of those experimental results have been replicated?

in defense of his assertion that "Science relies upon authority; not, as a practical day-to-day matter, falsification."

I had a go at it; you can see the result here. Briefly: I got fed up after two papers, but the two papers I chose, essentially at random, were readily verified by subsequent work building on the initial results, in exactly the way coturnix describes.